WOW !! MUCH LOVE ! SO WORLD PEACE !
Fond bitcoin pour l'amélioration du site: 1memzGeKS7CB3ECNkzSn2qHwxU6NZoJ8o
  Dogecoin (tips/pourboires): DCLoo9Dd4qECqpMLurdgGnaoqbftj16Nvp


Home | Publier un mémoire | Une page au hasard

 > 

The morphosyntax of ghomala' verbs: focus on inherent complément verbs and serial verb construction


par Corrine Minette FOKO MOKAM
Université de Yaoundé 1 - Master 2020
  

précédent sommaire suivant

Bitcoin is a swarm of cyber hornets serving the goddess of wisdom, feeding on the fire of truth, exponentially growing ever smarter, faster, and stronger behind a wall of encrypted energy

Conclusion

This chapter was devoted to Inherent Complement Verbs in Gh?maìlaì'. In this chapter, I have addressed their morphosyntactic properties. In doing so, their formal features as well as their semantics have been discussed. Being compositional or metaphorical, ICVs in the language understudy can be classified into regular and irregular. Looking at their morphosyntactic features, Gh?maìlaì' ICs can be pronominalized in certain discourse context. This is an indication that IC bears phi-features in this language as opposed to Ga (Korsah 2011) or Gungbe (Aboh 2015). It has also been shown that IC cannot be marked with a question feature in Gh?maìlaì'. As far as focalization is concerned, it has been argued that both the verb and its complement can be focused though the focused IC has a predicate focus reading. On the basis of these morphosyntactic properties, it have been claimed that Gh?maìlaì' ICVs are either intransitive or transitive. They purely merge under a functional position namely ?° that selects an abstract VP which licenses a bare NP. This NP incorporates to the abstract V. This functional behavior of verbs can also be mirrored in Serial Verb Constructions, the topic of the next chapter.

Chapter 5: Serial Verb Constructions (SVCs)

117

Introduction

The preceding chapter has addressed a type of verb construction wherein the verb and its complement are semantically tied such that it is difficult to state on whether the nominal constituent is an argument of the verb or otherwise. The present chapter deals with another type of verbal construction wherein there is more than one verb that function as a single predicate. This latter has been referred to as serial verb constructions, verbs series or verbal serialization in the literature. Their typology as well as their morphosyntactic properties in Gh?maìlaì' are investigated herein. In doing so, the chapter is structured as follows: section 1 distinguishes serial verb constructions from other types of multiverb constructions. Section 2 classifies serial verb constructions following their semantic functions. Section 3 tackles their morphosyntactic features and section 4 has to do with their derivation.

5.1. Serial Verb Constructions vs other multiverb constructions

This section aims at distinguishing serial verb constructions from other multiverb constructions that can be attested in Gh?maìlaì'. In this vein, formal properties that characterize serial verb constructions are addressed as well as diagnostics that differentiate SVCs from other multiverb structures.

5.1.1. General characteristics of SVCs

Aikhenvald (2006:1) defines a serial verb construction as a sequence of verbs which act together as a single predicate, without any overt marker of coordination, subordination, or syntactic dependency of any other sort. She proposes the following properties that are cross-linguistically useful in the recognition of a serial verb construction.

V' SVCs encode a single predicate reading. In other words, verbs which made up SVC function as a syntactic whole such that, most of the time, SVCs are translatable as single or coordinated predicates into non-serializing languages like English.

V' SVCs are monoclausal and do not allow makers of syntactic dependency on their components. More precisely, they are devoid of formal linking device such as one can argue that they are coordinate or subordinate structures. This property is a criterion that distinguishes SVC from coordination or subordination.

118

y' SVC has the intonational properties of monoverbal clause, and not of a sequence of clauses.

y' Verbs which form a SVC share tense, aspect, mood, modality, illocutionary force and polarity values. This implies that no independent choice or contrast in any of these categories is possible for the individual components of an SVC. They cannot be separately negated or focused.

y' SVCs encode one event. All verbs in an SVC form a close-knit structure perceived as a conceptual unit. As Lord (1974:196) points out, the verbs in SVC all refer to sub-parts or aspects of a single overall event.

y' SVCs share at least one argument. They do not allow duplicate roles, that is, they tend not to have two different agents, two themes or two instruments.

To see how these properties work in Gh?maìlaì', let us consider the following data:

(1)

a. Ta^laì k?Ì s?Ì? laÌ?t? aì

Tala PST2 come show 1SG.ACC «Tala came and showed me»

b. Ta^laì k?Ì v?Ì ?waÌ???Ì ?keì Tala PST2 write letter read «Tala wrote a letter and read it»

c. Ta^laì k?Ì l?Ì luÌ?gaì s?Ì?
Tala PST2 take bucket come «Tala bring a bucket»

In the data above, the verbs follow one another within the same clause as in (1a). There is no overt connector that links these verbs such that one can argue that they are instances of coordination or subordination. They represent a single predicate (1c); they form the core of a single clause. They share tense / aspect values as the past tense is marked once per SVC. They also share at least one argument, mostly the subject (Ta^laì).

5.1.2. Serial verb construction vs. coordinate structure

Coordinate structures are distinct from serial verb constructions by the presence of a coordinative marker that rules out a monoclausal interpretation. This suggests that, in coordinate

119

clauses, both subject and temporal feature can be overtly expressed and that core arguments need not be shared.

Moguo (2016: 271) does an inventory of Gh?maìlaì's coordinative markers. Accordingly, the following coordinators are attested in the language:

(2)

pû «and/with» k?Ì «or»

biì? «and» daÌ?g?Ì «but»

n?ì? «then»

Puì «and/with» is exclusively used only for NP coordination whereas others are used for VP coordination as well as sentence coordination. Puì, né? and biì? are cumulative coordinators; daÌ?g? is a restrictive one meanwhile coordinates alternative clauses as shown in the examples below:

(3)

a. Ta^laì pû Ba?ka^m k? tsù msé Tala COORD Bakam PST2 eat fufu corn «Tala and Bakam ate fufu corn»

b. Ta^laì k?Ì s?Ìk m-taìp n?ì? tiì Tala PST2 wash PL-shoe COORD sleep «Tala washed shoes and slept»

c. Ta^laì g?tiì v?Ì ?waÌ???Ì bi? ?keì ??msiì

Tala FUT2 write letter COORD read gospel

«Tala will write the letter and will read the gospel »

(4)

a. Ta^laì k?Ì s?Ìk m-taìp daÌ?g? e kaÌt? j????ì
Tala PST2 wash PL-shoe COORD 3SG PST2.NEG dry

«Tala washed the shoes but he didn't dry them»

b. Ta^laì g?tiì né mkoì k?Ì e g?tiì t?û? p?Ì
Tala FUT2 cook beans COORD 3SG FUT2 pound cocoyam

«Tala will cook beans or he will pound cocoyam».

In the examples above, the presence of the coordinative particles n?ì?, bi?, k?Ì and daÌ?g? rules out a monoclausal reading of these structures. Moreover, temporal features are expressed once in

120

cumulative structures (3) whereas in (4), each conjunct has its own tense marker. In (3c), the subject of the first conjunct controls that of the second. Given Baker (1989)'s generalization according to which covert coordination does exist in serializing languages, it can be argued that Gh?maìlaì' object sharing SVCs are not instances of covert coordination. The main evidence of this fact is that the object of an SVC structure can be extracted as shown in (5) below. This operation is impossible in coordinate structure given the coordinate structure constraint (Ross 1967).

(5)

a. Ta^lá k?Ì v?Ì ?waÌ???Ì ?keì

Tala PST2 write letter read

«Tala wrote a letter and read it»

b. A b? ?waÌ???Ì j?? Ta^lá k?Ì v?Ì _____ ?keì

It be letter REL Tala PST2 write read REL «It is the letter that Tala wrote and read»

5.1.3. Serial verb construction vs. Consecutivization

The main distinction between serial verb constructions and consecutive constructions relies on their intonational properties in Gh?maìlaì'. SVCs have a single intonation contour, while consecutive constructions have phonological break which corroborate with their interpretation as multi-clausal unit. Moreover, subject argument of each verb occurring in a consecutive structure can be overtly expressed as shown below:

(6)

a. Ta^lá g?tiì lu? si s?Ìk siìdj?? khiìm g?ìn?Ìm

Tala FUT2 wake up wash floor feed pig

«Tala will wake up, sweep the floor and feed the pig.»

b. N??n?^ k?Ì tû? ?j? pj? kwa?
Nono PST2 draw water 1PL.NOM carry «Nono drew water and we carried it»

5.1.4. Serial verb construction vs. subordinate structure

A subordinate clause is different from SVC by the presence of a subordinative particle which rules out a monoclausal interpretation. This indicates that in subordinate structures, the

121

subject and the temporal setting might be overtly expressed. Temporal features, aspectual value, negation as well as core arguments are not shared.

Gh?maìlaì' uses different strategies to encode subordination. It can be indicated either by nominalization within an infinitival clause with «to (in order)» or by various subordinative particles such as d3jégà «so that», «that», «until», pâ2 «as» as highlighted in the following data:

(7)

a. Ta^laì g3tiì y3 goì d?jâgâ é g3tiì kaìp pjà
Tala FUT2 go farm SUB 3SG.NOM FUT2 pick avocado «Tala will go to farm so that he will pick avocado»

b. Ba?kàm kà w? ?kw?ì gâ é g3tiì y3 go Bakam PST2 PROG think COMP 3SG FUT2 go farm

«Bakam was thinking that she will go to farm»

As shown in the data above, the presence of the subordinators d3jégé (7a) and (7b) eliminate a monoclausal interpretation of these sentences. Moreover, core arguments, temporal setting as well as aspectual feature are overtly expressed in each conjunct.

Let us consider the examples below that distinguish SVCs from subordination by infinitival construction.

(8)

a. Ta^laì kà s3Ì? là?t?

Tala PST2 come show 1SG.ACC «Tala came and showed me.»

b. Ta^laì kà s3Ì2 nâ là2t? aì
Tala PST2 come INF show 1SG.ACC « Tala came to show me.»

(9)a. Ta^laì g3tiì Y3 t fâ? m-taìp Tala FUT2 go search PL-shoe

«Tala will go and look for shoes.» b. Ta^laì g3tiì Y3 nâ t fâ? m-taìp Tala FUT2 go INF search PL-shoe

122

«Tala will go to look shoes.»

In (9) above, the same proposition is casted in a serial verb construction (8a and 9a) as opposed to subordination by infinitival construction (8b and 9b). Morphologically, the subordinate forms are reduced in that they do not inflect for tense. Besides, the significant distinction between these sentences is that the subordinate construction semantically indicates intentionality, meanwhile the serial verb construction expresses the idea of sequentiality.

5.2. Types of serial verb constructions

According to Aikhenvald (2006:21) and Kiebling (2011:30), in terms of their composition, serial verb constructions fall into two broad classes: symmetrical and asymmetrical SVCs. This opposition corresponds to Osam (2003)'s distinction between clause chaining serialization and integrated serial verb construction in Akan. It also matches with Foley and Olson (1985)'s dichotomy of core layer serialization vs. nuclear layer serialization. This section aims at classifying Gh?maìlaì' verb series on the basis of their composition following Aikhenvald (2006)'s repartition. In addition to their composition, SVCs are also classified following their semantic functions. In fact, SVCs are cross-linguistically used to express a wide range of functions amongst which direction and orientation, benefactive, comparative, modality, instrumental...etc.

5.2.1. Asymmetrical serial verb constructions

This class of SVCs may be made up of one verb from a large, open or unrestricted class and another from a semantically or grammatically restricted or closed class. As Aikhenvald (1999) claims , they denote a single event described by the verb from an unrestricted class. Following Durie (1997), the verb from an open class is referred to as a major verb whereas the term minor verb or coverb (Kiebling 2011:90) is used for the verb selected from a grammatically restricted class. The latter tends to get grammaticalized as it is the case of the Ewe verb na «give» which becomes a functional particle, namely a benefactive marker in this Kwa language (Ameka 2002:2). The minor verb provides a modificational specification to the whole construction. Semantic subclasses of asymmetrical SVCs in Gh?maìlaì' are addressed below.

5.2.1.1. Direction and orientation

As claimed by Aikhenvald (2006:22), this type of serial verb construction is extremely attested in serializing languages. Also referred to as deictic SVC (Givon 1991:139), this kind of serial verb construction is made up of a minor verb which is typically a verb of motion or

123

movement with the semantics of orientation. The following Gh?maìlaì' SVCs encode the idea of orientation.

(10)

a. Ta^laì g?tiì l?Ì dz?ì s?Ì?
Tala FUT2 take clothe come «Tala will bring clothe.»

b. Ta^laì g?tiì l?Ì luÌ?gaì g??

Tala FUT2 take bucket go

«Tala will take away the bucket.»

In the data above, the minor verbs are s32 «come» and g?? «go» whereas the major verb is «take». The motion verbs s?Ì? «come» and g?? «go» provide directional specification to the serial verb construction. Là s32 «take- come» means «bring» meanwhile là g?? «take-go» encodes «take away». Moreover, the motion verb g?? (??) has been grammaticalized in the language. In addition to its lexical status as in (10b) above, this verb is widely used as the root marker of future tenses. Indeed, when we observe the form of future tense markers in Gh?maìlaì', one may notice that the particle g? is common to all these future morphemes. What fundamentally changes is the particle that refers to the time frame as shown below:

Table 13 : Gh?maìlaì ' future tense markers

Markers

Segmentation

Time frame

Type of future

g???

g?-ya

few minutes or hours

Immediate future (FUT1)

g?tiì

g?-tI

a day or several days

Near future (FUT2)

g?t?w?ì

g?-tJwaì

some months or few years

Distant future (FUT3)

g?laì?

g?-lá?

undetermined time

Hypothetic future (FUT4)

5.2.1.2. Aspect and tense

Some asymmetrical serial verb constructions often encode aspectual meanings. Cross-linguistically, the minor verbs used in this type of verb series impart the semantics of progressive, habitual, continuative, or iterative meanings (see Aikhenvald 2006:185). Aikhenvald (2006:23) acknowledges that aspectual meanings expressed with SVCs may correlate with tense, but she have not found an example of an SVC used just for encoding tense. As it will be illustrated below,

124

Gh?maìlaì' used SVCs for imparting both aspect and tense. More precisely, Gh?maìlaì' native speakers use a serial verb construction to encode iterative meaning and to express future tense especially within imperative structures.

(11)

a. Ta^laì k?Ì kwiÌ? d?? ?kaìp?
Tala PST2 increase amount money

«Tala increased the amount of money.»

b. Ta^laì k?Ì kwiÌ? haì ?kaìp bi^ pû

Tala PST2 increase give money to 3PL.DAT «Tala again gave them money»

(12)

a. Ta^laì k?Ì loì??? tiì
Tala PST2 quickly sleep «Tala slept early.»

b. Po tiì s?Ìk mtaìp!
2PL.NOM sleep wash shoes «Wash the shoes tomorrow!»

(13)

a. Ta^laì k?Ì t?w?ì sim

Tala PST2 stay market

«Tala spent time in the market

b. t?w?ì kw?ì pa?

stay build house

«Build the house next year»

In (11a, 12a, 13a), the verbs kwiÌ? «increase», tiì «sleep» and t?wé «stay» are respectively used in a non-SVC structures. When they co-occur with another verb in the same clause, they encode new semantic function. In (11b), kwiÌ? is an aspectual marker; it is used to express the semantics of iterative. In (12b and13b), tiì and t?wé are respectively used as tense markers. They indicate the moment at which the action denoted by the following verbs will be done. As one may notice, the

125

verbs tI «sleep» and t?wé «stay» have been grammaticalized into future time frame particles (see the table in § 5.2.1 above).

5.2.1.3. Increasing valency

Certain kinds of asymmetrical serial verb construction are used as valency-increasing strategies. They are used to introduce various arguments as well as obliques. By doing so, they expand the argument structure of a given construction. Cross-linguistically, causatives, benefactives, instrumentals, and comitatives are encoded by serial verb constructions in serializing languages. As for Gh?maìlaì', causation and instrumentation are typically expressed by verb series. The verb of causation is generally the first verb just like the verb introducing instrument in instrumental series as shown below:

(14)

a. Ta^lá k?Ì y?Ì muì?? já? ?k?deì Tala PST2 make knife cut banana «Tala made the knife cuts banana»

b. Ta^lá k?Ì l?Ì d??m koì? th?ìpjaÌ

Tala PST2 take axe cut avocado tree

«Tala used an axe to cut avocado tree»

In the data above, the minor verbs are «make» and «take». Typical causative SVCs in Gh?maìlaì' involve the coverb «make»; thereby they are different from cause-effect serial verb constructions which are symmetrical series. The instrumental SVC in (14b) includes the verb «take». Besides, the instrument of V2 is the theme of V1. The verb «take» is also used to derive associative or comitative (Aikhenvald 2006:26) SVCs. In these structures, it gets the meaning «be with» and introduces the associative argument as shown in the example below:

(15)

a. Ta^lá k?Ì l?Ì mu^ foÌ s?Ì?
Tala PST2 take child chief come «Tala came with the prince.»

b. Ba?kám g?tiì l?Ì N?^n?? ?? go?

Bakam FUT2 take Nono go farm «Bakam will go to farm with Nono»

126

In some instrumental SVCs, the verb introducing the instrument describe the way in which the action of the other verb is performed as demonstrated below:

(16)

a. Ta^laì k?Ì l?Ì g?? pf?ì bap

Tala PST2 take strength eat meat

«Tala quickly ate the meat» b.Ta^laì k?Ì l?Ì kwè ??Ìm

Tala PST2 take joke speak

«Tala jokingly spoke»

Other types of valence increasing serial verb constructions are benefactive ones. In the language under study, benefactive SVCs include a verb of transfer and a preposition that introduces the beneficiary or recipient argument as illustrated by the data below:

(17)

a. Ta^laì k?Ì joì b?? haì bi^ N?^n?? Tala PST2 buy pot give to Nono «Tala bought Nono a pot»

b. N?^n?? k?Ì v?Ì ?waÌ???Ì t?jaÌ bi^ maìp e

Nono PST2 write letter send to mother 3SG.1.POSS «Nono send her mother a letter»

The minor verbs in (17) above are the verbs of transfer t?jaÌ «send» and haì «give». The preposition bi^ «to» indicates that the following DPs, namely N?^n?? and maìp e «her mother», are the beneficiaries of the action described by the first verbs.

5.2.1.4. Comparison

Asymmetrical serial verb constructions are also used to encode comparative and superlative meanings. This kind of verb series always involve verbs with a meaning of «exceed» as minor verb. In a West Chadic language spoken in Nigeria like Goemai, the verb imparting the comparative meaning to the whole construction is translated as «surpass». This is illustrated in (18) below:

127

(18) kuma f'yer ma ni

also become.big (SG) surpass 3SG

«And (he) has grown bigger than him» (Hellwig 2006:101)
As far as Gh?maìlaì' is concerned, serial verb constructions are used to mark comparison. As a matter of fact, verb series with comparative meaning in the language under study include the verb t?jè «surpass» as V2. Moreover, this verb has been grammaticalized into a comparative marker. The following data (19) are instances of comparative SVCs whereas those in (19) show that the minor verb t?jè serves as a comparative maker elsewhere.

(19)

a. N?^n?? k?Ì tiì t?j?Ì Ta^laì
Nono PST2 sleep surpass Tala «Nono slept more than Tala»

b. Ba?ka^m twaÌ t?j?Ì N?^n??
Bakam.PRS be beautiful surpass Nono «Bakam is more beautiful than Nono».

(20)

a. Ta^laì k?Ì f?Ì? m?Ìtwa^ t?j?Ì taìp e
Tala PST2 drive car COM father 3SG.1.POSS

«Tala drove car more than his father»

b. N?^n?? g?tiì tsù ms?? t?j?Ì p?ì

Nono FUT2 eat fufu corn COM pounded cocoyam

«Nono will eat more fufu corn than pounded cocoyam»

As it has been shown in this subsection, Gh?maìlaì' has enriched his functional particles stock with the grammaticalization of minor verbs that occur in serial verb constructions. Some TAM particles as well as the comparative marker in Gh?maìlaì' are the outcomes of the grammaticalization process that targets some verbs.

5.2.2. Symmetrical serial verb constructions

Unlike asymmetrical serial verb constructions, this class of SVCs is made up of verbs selected from unrestricted classes. According to Kiebling (2011:37), symmetrical SVCs indicate an immediate succession of events or actions in a sequence which is characterized by tight logical

128

and temporal coherence. In this vein, the order of components tend to be iconic, mirroring the temporal sequence of subevents. Following Aikhenvald (2006:22), all the components of symmetrical SVCs have equal status since none of them determines the semantic or syntactic properties as a whole. Semantic subclasses of symmetrical SVCs in Gh?maìlaì' are discussed in the following lines.

5.2.2.1. Sequential serial verb constructions

Also referred to as consecutive serial verb constructions (Stewart 2001), the order of components follows the temporal sequence of subevents described by each verb of the series as illustrated below:

(21)

a. Ta^laì k?Ì n?ì bap pf?ì Tala PST2 cook meat eat «Tala cooked meat and ate it»

b. N?^n?? g?tiì ??Ìm Ba?ka^m pû?

Nono FUT2 catch Bakam beat

«Nono will catch Bakam and beat her»

In the above examples, the verbs are linked by a temporal sequence in which the event encoded by the first verb is performed before the one of the second verb. Accordingly, V1 and V2 express consecutive events. In other words, the action described by the second verb closely follows the one that is expressed by the first verb. Data in (21) also show that there are two transitive verbs that have one shared object sandwiched between them. Moreover, no overt pronoun appears after the second verb. It is relevant to note that this type of sharing has been a very popular object of generative inquiry in serializing languages (Baker 1989, Collins 1997, Hale 1991, Hiraiwa & Bodomo 2008, and Aboh 2009).

5.2.2.2. Cause-effect or resultative serial verb constructions

This kind of symmetrical SVCs have an iconic component order. Indeed, the verb of causation always precedes the verb that refers to the effect or the result. The following constructions are instances of cause-effect SVCs in Gh?maìlaì'.

(22) 129

a. Ta^laì k?Ì t??Ì muì l?Ì

Tala PST2 beat child cry

«Tala made child cry by beating him»

b. N?^n?? k? t?aìm k?ì s?Ì

Nono PST2 hit plate split

«Nono shattered the plate»

As it is shown in the data above, the action expressed by the second verb is caused by that of first verb. In (22a) for example, the child cried as Tala beat him as well as the plate is shattered because Nono hits it. Furthermore, the object of the first verb is the subject of V2. The verbs involved in the series have a relation of causality, thereby are similar to causative SVCs. As Aikhenvald (2006:16) pointed out, causative SVCs are asymmetrical since the verb that refers to causation in the series comes from a closed set of transitive verbs; while cause-effect SVCs tend to be symmetrical. They are switch-function SVCs. As for Gh?maìlaì', the two types of SVCs are iconic, that is the verbs encoding causation always precedes the verbs referring to the consequence. Causative SVCs involve the verb ?? «make», while cause-effect SVCs include any transitive verb.

5.2.2.3. Manner serial verb constructions

In this kind of symmetrical SVCs, the first verb describes the way in which the action expressed by the second is performed. The first verb functions as a modifier of the action encoded by the second verb. So, the first verb is the modifying verb whereas the second verb is the main verb. As far as Gh?maìlaì' is concerned, the language uses two strategies to convey manner serialization, namely increasing valency strategy and simple strategy. Increasing valency refers to the fact that some instrumental series in the language are also used to impart manner serialization as it has been shown in (14) above (see § 5.2.1.3). The other strategy is the raison d'être of the data in (23) below:

(23)

a. Ta^laì k? j?ìmn?ì fa?aì Tala PST2 be slow work «Tala worked slowly»

130

b. Ta^laì g?tiì lo??? pf?ì bap

Tala FUT2 be quick eat meat

«Tala will quickly eat the meat».

Most of the first verbs appearing in manner serialization in Gh?maìlaì' have been identified as

adverbs elsewhere (Foba 2015:94).

5.3. Morphosyntactic properties of Gh?maìlaì' SVCs

This section aims at discussing the morphosyntactic features of serial verb constructions in Gh?maìlaì'. In doing so, the expression and marking of grammatical categories within SVCs as well as their interaction with question formation and focalization are also addressed.

5.3.1. Marking of grammatical categories

The monoclausality of serial verb constructions in Gh?maìlaì' can be mirrored through the single

marking of verbal categories such as tense, aspect and negation. It can also be captured through some

morphosyntactic operations, namely nominalization and subordination.

5.3.1.1. Tense

These functional categories are marked once per serial verb construction in Gh?maìlaì' as it

can be noticed in the following data:

(24)a. N?^n?? g?tiì l?Ì jw?ìtsù s?Ì?

Nono FUT2 take food come

«Nono will bring food.»

b.*N?^n?? l?Ì jw?ìts?ì g?tiì s?Ì?

Nono take food FUT2 come

«Nono will bring food.»

c. *Ta^laì k?Ì joì mtaìp k?Ì fiÌ?

Tala PST2 buy shoes PST2 sell

«Tala bought shoes and sold them

As it is shown in (24) above, single marking of tense is attested in the language under study. In (24a), the two verbs, «take» and s?Ì ? «come», share the same tense marker «g?tiì», though this latter appears just before the first verb. This tense particle has scope over the two verbs of the construction. Data in (24b andc) rule out the possibility for the tense marker to be marked either on the second verb or on each components of the series.

131

5.3.1.2. Aspect

This verbal category is expressed diversely within Gh?maìlaì' verb series. Single and

concordant markings of aspect are both attested in the language. This property can be mirrored in

the data below:

(25) a. Ta^laì k?Ì joì mtaìp w?ì fiÌ?

Tala PST2 buy shoes PROG sell

«Tala was buying shoes and was selling them»

b. Ba?ka^m k?Ì w?ì l?Ì ?kaìp g??
Bakam PST2 PROG take money go «Bakam was taking away the money»

c. Ba?ka^m k?Ì w?ì né msé w?ì dz?ì
Bakam PST2 PROG cook fufu corn PROG eat «Bakam was cooking fufu corn and was eating it.»

d. * Ba?ka^m k?Ì w?ì l?Ì ?kaìp w?ì g??
Bakam PST2 PROG take money PROG go «Bakam was taking away the money»

Data in (25a and b) show that aspect can be marked once per serial verb construction. The aspect particle can appear either on the first verb or on the second one and has the scope over the two verbs. The construction in (25c) demonstrates that each verb in Gh?maìlaì' SVCs can have its own aspect marker. However, this property is restricted to consecutive SVCs as (25d) implies. Moreover, (25a) indicates that functional elements can occur between V1 and V2. This will be significant when the issue of derivation of SVCs will be addressed.

5.3.1.3. Negation

Just like tense, negation receives a single marking per serial verb construction in Gh?maìlaì'. As claimed by Kari (2003), verbs in series are not independently negated such that any attempt to negate serial verbs independently , like when the verbs appear in simple constructions from which SVC derives, renders them illegible. Let us observe the following data:

(26)a. N?^n?? t?^ g?tiì l?ì jw?ìts?ì s?Ì? p?ì

Nono NEG FUT2 take food come NEG «Nono will not bring food»

132

b. Ta^laì kaÌt? mtaìp fiÌ? ( p?ì)

Tala PST2.NEG buy shoes sell NEG «Tala did not buy shoes and sell them.» (27) a. *N?^n?? g?tiì l?Ì jw?ìts?ì t?^ s?Ì2 p?ì

Nono FUT2 take food NEG come NEG «Nono will not bring food.»

b. *Ta^laì kaÌt? jó mtaìp p?ì kaÌt? fiÌ? p?ì

Tala PST2.NEG buy shoes NEG PST2.NEG sell NEG

«Tala did not buy shoes and sell them.»

The data in (26) above reveal that negation is marked once in the serial verb construction by the discontinous morphemes t?^ ...pé. The first particle appears immediately before the verb while the second occurs at the sentence final position. Data in (27) show that it is impossible for each verb of the series to have its own negative morpheme (27b) just like the negative particle cannot appear on the second verb (27a). Even though the first particle is placed before the first verb, it has scope over the other verb of the series.

5.3.1.4. Nominalization

Nominalization is a word class changing derivation process which generally turns verbs into nouns. It was been argued in chapter 3 that the infinitive particle also acts as a nominalizer in Gh?maìlaì' since the infinitive form is a nominalized form of the verb (see § 3.2.1 above). It is important to note that, in the language under study, verbs are also turned into nouns via reduplication. However, this process is restricted to the derivation of agents. As for serial verb constructions, they receive a single marking of nominalizer per construction as it can be mirrored in the data below.

(28)a. Ta^laì k?Ì v?Ì ?waÌ???Ì ?keì

Tala PST2 write letter read

«Tala wrote a letter and read it»

b. Juì n?ì-v?Ì ?waÌ2??Ì ké k?Ì t?j?Ì Ba?ka^m
2SG.3.POSS INF-write letter read PST2 surpass Bakam «YOUR writing and reading of the letter surpassed Bakam»

133

c. *Jû n?ì-v?Ì ?waÌ???Ì n?ì- ke^ k?Ì t?j?Ì Ba?kam

2SG.3.POSS INF-write letter INF-read PST2 surpass Bakam «YOUR writing and reading of the letter surpassed Bakam»

d. *Jû v?Ì ?waÌ???Ì n?ì-ke^ k?Ì t?j?Ì Ba?ka^m

2SG.3.POSS write letter INF-read PST2 surpass Bakam

«YOUR writing and reading of the letter surpassed Bakam»

As shown in the above data, serial verb constructions are nominalized the way monoverbal constructions are. The nominative morpheme appears on the first component and has scope over the second component. As example (28c) implies, each verb cannot have its own nominalizer. Similarly, it is impossible to see the nominalizer occurs on the second verb (28d). This nominalization process for SVCs is another evidence of their monoclausal status.

5.3.1.5. Subordination

The monoclausal status of serial verb constructions can also be captured in their syntactic behavior especially within subordination. As Rose (2009) observed, serial verb constructions can form a unique predicate of a subordinate clause introduced by a single relativizer or subordinator, just like any single verb. The following examples are instances of subordinate clauses including SVCs.

(29) a Mtaìp mj? Ta^laì k?Ì joì fiÌ? kaìt? pu?
shoes 4REL Tala PST2 buy sell REL PST2.NEG be good «Shoes that Tala bought and sold were not good.»

b. N?^n?? kwè g?ì Ba?ka^m g?tiì l?Ì jw?ìts?ì s?Ì? Nono think COMP Bakam FUT2 take food come «Nono thinks that Bakam will bring food.»

c. N?ì-j?^mn?ì faì? j? Ta^laì kuÌ? t?^ g?laì? kwi^t? p?ì

INF-be slow work 3REL Tala like REL NEG FUT4 help NEG

working slowly that Tala likes will not be helpful

«The fact that Tala likes to work slowly will never be helpful.»

The examples above show that subordinative particles are marked once per construction involving

verb series. In (29a), the sandwiched object of the SVC is relativized, a further evidence that

Gh?maìlaì' SVCs are not instances of covert coordination. In (29b), an SVC is part of the

134

subordinate clause introduced by the complementizer gé, while in (29c) it is the verb series which is relativized via nominalization.

5.3.2. Question and Focus marking

In Gh?maìlaì' just like in Ewe (Ameka 2006:140), components of serial verb constructions can be questioned and focused separately. As for question marking, yes/no questions are signaled by the utterance of final particles a or l?ì (Tala 2015: 68). Even though these interrogative particles are marked once per SVC and they appear at the sentence final position, they can have scope over all the components of the series. Their scope could also be restricted on only one verb of the series. Let us observe the data in (30) below:

(30)a. Ta^laì k?Ì joì mtaìp fiÌ?

Tala PST2 buy shoes sell

«Tala bought shoes and sold them»

b. Ta^laì k?Ì joì mtaìp fiÌ? a ?

Tala PST2 buy shoes sell QM

«Did Tala buy shoes and sell them?»

c. Ta^laì k?Ì s?Ìk mtaìp fiÌ?

Tala PST2 wash shoes sell

«Tala washed shoes and sold them.»

d. Ta^laì k?Ì joì mtaìp s?Ìk

Tala PST2 buy shoes wash

«Tala bought shoes and washed them.»

The question in (30b) above can be answered in several fashions, indicating thereby components on which the question particle has scope. It can be answered by «yes, Tala did buy and sell» or «no, Tala didn't buy and sell» where the scope of the question is over both verbs. However, it can also be answered with either (30c), implying question scope over the second verb, or (30d), implying question scope over the first verb. Moreover, components of SVCs can be individually questioned using the content question strategy as shown below:

(31)a. Ta^laì k?Ì joì mtaìp fiÌ?

Tala PST2 buy shoes sell

«Tala bought shoes and sold them»

b.

135

Ta^laì k?Ì jó mtaìp ??Ì k?ì ?
Tala PST2 buy shoes do what ? «What did Tala buy shoes and do?»

c. Ta^laì k?Ì ??Ì k?ì fiÌ??

Ta^laì PST2 do what sell

«What did Tala do and sell?

In the above examples, content questions are marked by the interrogative determiner . To question a VP or a happening, the wh-phrase «what» and the functional verb «do» are employed. The verbs forming the SVC in (31a) can each be questioned, as in (31b) and (31c). As far as focus is concerned, each component of a serial verb construction can be focused. In Gh?maìlaì', verb focus is realized via predicate doubling. The two copies of the verb occur IP-internally and convey a contrastive reading. The following data are instances of focalization of SVC components.

(32) a. Ba?ka^m k?Ì n?ì ms?ì tsù
Bakam PST2 cook fufu corn eat

«Bakam cooked fufu corn and ate it.»

b. Ba?ka^m k?Ì n?ì msé n?ì tsù

Bakam PST2 cook fufu corn cook eat

«Bakam COOKED fufu corn and ate (as opposed to roasted

c. Ba?ka^m k?Ì n?ì msé tsé tsé

Bakam PST2 cook fufu corn eat eat

«Bakam cooked fufu corn and ATE it (as opposed to sold)»

d. *Ba?ka^m k?Ì n?ì tsé msé dzé

Bakam PST2 cook eat fufu corn eat

«Bakam cooked fufu and ATE it.»

As shown in the examples above, each verb of the series can be focused. Verb focusing in Gh?maìlaì' is derived mainly by duplication of the verb IP-internally. The complement of the clause is thereby sandwiched between the two copies of the verb. What particulary happens with an SVC is that the focalization of the first verb exhibits this pattern; the object is effectively sandwiched between the two verb copies as in (32b) above. This is not the case with the second verb focusing

136

where the two verb copies are contiguous (32c) and any insertion of the object between the copies leads to ungrammaticality (32d). This could be an indication that object sharing in Gh?maìlaì' SVCs is asymmetrical.

5.3.3. Argument structure

It has been argued that serial verb constructions are monoclausal and represent a single predicate. By being so, they share at least one argument, mostly the external argument. This subsection takles the transitivity properties of SVCs components.

Most of the verbs that occur in a serial verb constructions generally share the same Agent. This is the case of some SVCs in Gh?maìlaì' which generally combines a two-place predicate verb with an intransitive one. This combination does not rend the argument structure of the whole clause more complex than that of one of its component. For example, the verbs occurring in the SVC in (33) below have the following subcategorization frames: «take» c-selects two XP and «come» c-selects one XP.

(33) Ta^lá g?tiì l?Ì dz?ì s?Ì?

Tala FUT2 take clothe come

«Tala will bring clothe.»

As one may notice, the argument structure of this clause is not the sum of the arguments of each component. This SVC is a two-place predicate clause selecting Ta^laì as Agent and dzé «clothe» as Theme. SVCs combining an intransitive verb with a transitive one are two-place predicate constructions.

Similarly, there are some SVCs which, in addition to the external argument, share an internal argument. These are bona fide serial verb constructions (SVCs) because they obey the Argument-Sharing Hypothesis. Accordingly, the Argument Sharing Hypothesis states that, in a serial verb construction, V1 and V2 must share an internal argument (Collins 1997:463). The following data are instances of SVCs sharing an internal argument.

(34) a. Ta^lá k?Ì l?Ì d??m koì? th?ìpjaÌ
Tala PST2 take axe cut avocado tree «Tala used an axe to cut avocado tree»

b. Ta^lá k?Ì y?Ì muì?? já? ?k?deì
Tala PST2 make knife cut banana «Tala made the knife cuts banana»

137

c. Ta^laì k?Ì jó b?? haì bi^ N?^n??

Tala PST2 buy pot give to Nono

«Tala bought Nono a pot»

d. N?^n?? g?tiì ??Ìm Ba?ka^m pû?

Nono FUT2 catch Bakam beat

«Nono will catch Bakam and beat her»

Each SVC above shares an internal argument. In (34a), the theme of V1 (l?Ì «take») is the instrument of V2 (koì? «cut»). By the same token, the theme of V1 in (34b) is the agent of V2. In (34c), V1 and V2 share the same theme whereas in (34d) they share the same patient. Internal argument sharing SVCs have been of particular theoretical interest because of the issue of how to formally represent object sharing (Baker 1989, Hale 1991, Collins 1997 and Aboh 2009).

5.4. Derivation of serial verb constructions

Most of the propositions on how to formally represent serial verb constructions `structure relies on the argument sharing hypothesis. These proposals posit the argument sharing hypothesis as a necessary condition on serialization and argue for a serializing parameter. This parameter is formulated by Collins (1997:493) as follows: «I (tense) can license multiple Vs».

In this vein, Baker (1989)'s double-headed structure with a ternary-branching structure in which the object is shared by the two verbs (see § 1.3.1 above) cannot account for the structure of object-sharing SVCs in Gh?maìlaì'. Indeed, apart from being ruled out by modern approaches to phrase structure, the syntactic behavior of Gh?maìlaì' SVCs when they are focused clearly shows that object sharing is asymmetrical in narrow syntax. More precisely, as highlighted by the data in (32) above, the direct object can only be pied-piped with V1 in Gh?maìlaì' under predicate doubling. Baker (1989)'s double-headed structure just like Hale (1991)'s VP-adjunction structure seem to be more appropriate for symmetric object-sharing structures like in DaÌgaìaìrè (Hiraiwa and Bodomo 2008).

Similarly, Collins (1997)'s VP-shell structure is based on the mediation of object-sharing by an empty category namely pro, to which the second verb assigns its semantic role and it is coindexed with the object of the first verb. However, there is no evidence of the existence of an empty category within Gh?maìlaì' SVCs. Morever, the serializing parameter as formulated by Collins (1997) stipulates that I (tense) can license multiple Vs not multiple Is. As a matter of fact,

138

it has been shown that I-type functional elements that expresses aspect specifications can occur between V1 and V2 (see example (25) in § 5.3.1.2 above). Just like in Edoì, certain middle-field adverbs which Stewart (1998) treats as heads, can appear between the shared object and V2 in Gh?maìlaì' as shown below:

(35) a. Ba?ka^m k?Ì né msé tJaijtaì dz?ì

Bakam PST2 cook fufu corn well eat

«Bakam cooked fufu corn and ate it well.»

b. Ta^laì g?tiì joì mtaìp laÌ gy? fiÌ?

Tala FUT2 buy shoes quickly sell

«Tala will buy shoes and quickly sell them»

These examples show that any derivation based on argument sharing hypothesis cannot account for Gh?maìlaì' SVCs structures. Thus, Aboh (2009)'s proposal according to which the argument sharing hypothesis is not a necessary condition on serialization will be adopted.

Aboh (2009) proposes an analysis of serial verb construction which is not based on argument sharing hypothesis. Indeed, Aboh argues that the space between V1 and V2 involves more syntactic positions than previously assumed because it may involve tense, aspect markers, adverbs and clitic pronoun. He therefore proposes an analysis in which, given the temporal iconicity condition, the first verb merges as a functional (verbal) element within the extended projection of the lexical verb (the second verb) that introduces the arguments. The data in (25) is an evidence that there is an aspect head between V1 and V2. Moreover, the examples in (35) above show that adverbs can occur between V1 and V2. Following Cinque (1999)'s analysis of adverbs as expressions of functional projections namely AspPs, these examples are an evidence of the existence of an aspect position between V1 and V2. On the basis of what has been exposed above, the following tree diagram is the representation of the construction in (35a).

(36) 139

TP

Spec T'

Ba?ka^m T AspP
k?Ì Spec Asp'

Ba?ka^m Asp FP

n?ì F AspP

Spec Asp'

ms?ì Asp ?P
t???t?ì Spec ?'

Ba?ka^m ? VP

dzé V NP dz?ì

ms?ì

The derivation of the instrumental SVCs in (37a) is represented in (37b) below:

(37) a. Ta^laì g?tiì l?Ì d??m lo??? koì? th?ìpjaÌ
Tala FUT2 take axe quickly cut avocado tree «Tala will use an axe to cut avocado tree»

b. TP

Spec T'

Ta^laì T AspP

g?tiì Spec Asp'

Ta^laì Asp FP

l?Ì F AspP

Spec Asp'

d??m Asp ?P
lo??? Spec ?'

Ta^laì ? ?PINST

koì? Spec ?INST'

d??m ?INST VP

koì? V NP

koì? th?ìpjaÌ

140

In the above tree diagrams, V1 is merged in a functional projection, the higher AspP. In this position, it doesn't have an internal theta-role to assign but selects for complement within which the object is being licensed. In (37b), the instrument is introduced by the lower ?P and it latter move to the specifier position of the lower AspP. The main issue now is how to account for the structure of focused SVCs. This will be the object of future inquiry.

précédent sommaire suivant






Bitcoin is a swarm of cyber hornets serving the goddess of wisdom, feeding on the fire of truth, exponentially growing ever smarter, faster, and stronger behind a wall of encrypted energy








"Nous devons apprendre à vivre ensemble comme des frères sinon nous allons mourir tous ensemble comme des idiots"   Martin Luther King